Threesology Research Journal
Tripartite Aspects of Various Subjects 27




Series Contents 1, Chapters 1-9
Series Contents 2, Chapters 10-18
Series Contents 3, Chapters 19-27
Series Contents 3, Chapters 28-36
Aspects 1 Aspects 2 Aspects 3 Aspects 4 Aspects 5 Aspects 6 Aspects 7 Aspects 8 Aspects 9
Aspects 10 Aspects 11 Aspects 12 Aspects 13 Aspects 14 Aspects 15 Aspects 16 Aspects 17 Aspects 18
Aspects 19 Aspects 20 Aspects 21 Aspects 22 Aspects 23 Aspects 24 Aspects 25 Aspects 26 Aspects 27
Aspects 28 Aspects 29 Aspects 30 Aspects 31 Aspects 32 Aspects 33 Aspects 34 Aspects 35 Aspects 36

Flag Counter
Visitors as of Sept. 3rd, 2025


On this page in order of appearance:

  1. Dichotomies and Trichotomies inherent in IQ tests: [1]
    • This section is followed by the information culled from the "Deep Dive" option.
    • I then follow this up with the internet posed question "What are some criticisms of using dichotomous scoring in IQ tests?":
  2. What are some criticisms of using dichotomous scoring in IQ tests?: [2]
  3. What are some criticisms of using trichotomous scoring in IQ tests?: [3]
    • Note: this AI response moves towards historical generalizations levied against IQ testing methods, and does not remain explicitly focused on a review of trichotomous scoring methodology.
    • The follow-up "deeper dive" option is a reiterated review along the same lines of critiquing trichotomy with a dichotomous mindset and illustrated array of responding.
    • There is a recurring rather subjective criteria of confusing determining a person's level of intelligence with respect to answering given questions correctly and measuring a person's cognitive strategy as an excuse against a person achieving a poor score. Indeed what are we trying to measure?:
      • A person's knowledge of a given set of information which may be offered by providing contrasts?
      • A person's individual strategy as the primary goal of assessment?
      • A person's strategy and knowledge base?
      • A person's ability to guess at a given test session?
      • A person's behavioral adaptability to a monitored setting of expectations?
      • A person's inclination to set themselves up to portray a positive or negative assessment of themselves, where the test is thus their proof for defining how good or bad they are; so as to provide a desired reward or punishment for a given emotional state?
    • This assessment uses one model of trichotomy to present information based upon its 3-patterned structured format of "correct, incorrect, or partially correct", which raises multiple questions of partiality and might as well be labeled "yes, no, maybe", or some other syllogistically portrayed convention, which does not disclose the frequently overlooked presence of a embellished dichotomy falsely labeled as a trichotomy.
  4. Tripartite Aspects of the 3 historical puzzles used to test rationality: [4] The AI response is not incorrect, since I should have been more explicit in my query while thinking about the "Cognitive Resource test" (CRT). The battery of three puzzles by Shane Fredrick are:
    • Suppose that a baseball and a baseball bat together cost $1.10, and that the dollar costs a dollar more than the baseball. How much does the baseball cost? Answer: 5 cents.
    • If it takes 5 machines 5 minutes to make 5 widgets, how long would it take 100 machines to make 100 widgets? Answer: 5 minutes.
    • In a lake there is a patch of lily pads. Every day the patch doubles in size. If it takes 48 days to cover the entire lake, how long would it take for the patch to cover half the lake? Answer: 47 days.


[1] Internet query: "Dichotomies and Trichotomies inherent in IQ tests"

AI (overview) response: Dichotomies and trichotomies inherent in IQ tests and their underlying theories refer to how intelligence is conceptually divided into two or three categories, and how test questions and scoring methods reflect this division. These divisions influence both the structure of the tests and the interpretation of their results.

Dichotomies

Dichotomous test items

At the most basic level, many IQ test items rely on dichotomous scoring, where a response is coded as either correct or incorrect. This binary classification is used for item types such as:
Multiple-choice questions: Selecting the correct option from a list.
  • True/false statements: Evaluating the accuracy of a given sentence.
  • Scored short answers: A response is either fully correct or not.
  • This scoring method is a simplifying convention that facilitates test construction and evaluation but can overlook the nuances of a test-taker's reasoning.

General (g) and specific (s) factors

  The earliest and most influential dichotomy in intelligence theory is Charles Spearman's two-factor theory.
 
  • General factor (g): A central, overarching cognitive ability that influences performance across all mental tasks. Factor analysis of IQ test results consistently shows a positive correlation between scores on different sub-tests, which Spearman argued reflects this underlying (g) factor.
  • Specific factors (s): Unique abilities specific to particular tasks, such as verbal or mathematical skills, that account for the variation not explained by (g).

Fluid and crystallized intelligence

Building on Spearman's work, psychologist Raymond Cattell proposed a dichotomy of general intelligence.
 
  • Fluid intelligence (Gf): The capacity to think logically, reason abstractly, and solve novel problems. It is considered independent of learned knowledge.
  • Crystallized intelligence (Gc): The accumulation of knowledge, facts, and skills that are acquired throughout a lifetime.

Trichotomies

Robert Sternberg's triarchic theory

Psychologist Robert Sternberg argued that intelligence is a broader concept than what conventional IQ tests measure, proposing three distinct facets.
  • Analytical intelligence: The ability to analyze and evaluate information, similar to the traditional concept of academic intelligence.
  • Creative intelligence: The ability to generate novel ideas and react adaptively to new situations.
  • Practical intelligence: The ability to apply knowledge and skills to solve real-world problems. This is often referred to as "street smarts".

Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory

A modern and widely accepted framework, the CHC theory integrates the work of Cattell, Horn, and Carroll into a three-stratum hierarchical model. It is a complex taxonomy, but its highest levels form a trichotomy:
  • Stratum III (g): A single, general intelligence factor at the top of the hierarchy.
  • Stratum II (Broad abilities): Approximately 10 broad abilities below (g), such as fluid intelligence (Gf)), crystallized intelligence (Gc), and general memory and learning.
  • Stratum I (Narrow abilities): Over 70 specific abilities that fall under the broader factors. This model is influential in the construction of modern IQ tests like the Woodcock-Johnson and Wechsler scales. 

Implications for IQ testing

These theoretical dichotomies and trichotomies manifest in IQ testing in several ways:
  • Hierarchical scoring: Modern IQ tests typically produce a single overall score (representing (g) in the CHC model), along with separate scores for specific cognitive domains (representing broader abilities like (Gf) and (Gc).
  • Item type diversity: To capture these different facets, tests use a variety of item formats that go beyond a simple correct/incorrect dichotomy. This includes performance-based subtests, picture completion, and other novel-problem-solving tasks.
  • Conceptual limitations: Critics argue that even the most advanced trichotomous models fail to capture the full scope of human intelligence, such as emotional and social intelligence, creativity, and practical judgment.
  • Biases: The very nature of a dichotomous correct/incorrect score can be affected by cultural, linguistic, and socioeconomic biases, potentially measuring familiarity with test formats or cultural knowledge rather than pure intellectual ability.

 

This next section represents the information from the "Dive Deeper" option (Opera browser)

AI response: While modern intelligence tests are built on complex, hierarchical models, the foundational theories of intelligence and the scoring of individual items often rely on dichotomous and trichotomous frameworks. Dichotomies and trichotomies are inherent in IQ testing on both the micro and macro levels, from the binary scoring of specific test items to the broader classification of cognitive abilities.

Dichotomies in IQ testing

Micro-level: Item scoring

At the item level, many questions on IQ tests use dichotomous scoring, meaning a response is scored as either correct or incorrect. This is a simple binary distinction.
  • Selected-response items: True/False and multiple-choice questions are scored dichotomously. For example, on a multiple-choice item, a test-taker's response is either the correct answer or an incorrect one.
  • Constructed-response items: Even short-answer items that only have one acceptable solution can be scored dichotomously. The answer is either right or wrong.

Macro-level: Theories of intelligence

The overarching theoretical models used to structure IQ tests contain major dichotomies that divide intelligence into two distinct types.
  • Fluid vs. Crystallized Intelligence (Gf-Gc): Proposed by Raymond Cattell and foundational to the modern Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory, this is a core dichotomy in modern intelligence theory.
    • Fluid Intelligence (Gf): The ability to reason and solve new, abstract problems without relying on previously learned knowledge. This is often considered a more innate cognitive capacity.
    • Crystallized Intelligence (Gc): The ability to use skills, knowledge, and experience accumulated over time through education and cultural exposure.
  • Verbal vs. Non-Verbal Abilities: Early intelligence models, such as older versions of the Wechsler scales, often relied on a simple split between
    • verbal and performance-based abilities. While modern tests have expanded on this, the distinction remains foundational.
    • Verbal Scales: Measure language-based skills, such as vocabulary and verbal comprehension.
    • Non-Verbal Scales: Assess visual-spatial and reasoning skills that do not require language.

Trichotomies in IQ testing

Macro-level: Higher-order models

More sophisticated models, like the CHC theory, move beyond simple dichotomies to a more nuanced hierarchical structure that can be viewed as a trichotomy of three strata.
  • Stratum III (General Intelligence): At the top is the general intelligence factor (g), which represents an overall measure of cognitive ability.
  • Stratum II (Broad Abilities): The middle layer consists of broad cognitive abilities like Gf and Gc. The current Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-V) uses five index scores based on CHC theory, including Fluid Reasoning, Visual-Spatial, and Working Memory, which represent these broad abilities.
  • Stratum I (Narrow Abilities): The lowest layer consists of over 70 narrow abilities, which are the specific cognitive tasks that load onto the broader abilities. For example, the "verbal comprehension" broad ability is comprised of narrow abilities like vocabulary knowledge and verbal reasoning.

Wechsler scales structure

The factor structure of earlier Wechsler tests also demonstrated trichotomies. For example, factor analysis of the WISC-R revealed a three-factor structure:
  1. Verbal Comprehension
  2. Perceptual Organization
  3. Freedom from Distractibility (a factor that was later re-conceptualized in subsequent test editions).

Why these divisions matter

The presence of dichotomies and trichotomies is central to how IQ tests are constructed, interpreted, and understood.
  • Diagnostic specificity: For a clinician, understanding a test-taker's strengths and weaknesses across different dimensions (e.g., verbal vs. non-verbal or fluid vs. crystallized abilities) is more useful than a single, global IQ score. A test-taker might have an average Full-Scale IQ but struggle with a specific area like visual-spatial reasoning, which the more detailed factor scores can reveal.
  • Conceptual refinement: The evolution from simple dichotomous models (like Verbal vs. Performance) to hierarchical trichotomous models (like CHC) reflects a more nuanced, research-backed understanding of intelligence. This allows psychologists to assess a wider range of cognitive functions with greater precision.
  • Test item development: Psychometricians use item response theory (IRT) to develop and analyze test questions. A fundamental part of IRT is understanding if a test item is dichotomous (correct/incorrect) or polytomous (partially correct), which influences how test difficulty and reliability are assessed.


[2] Internet query: "What are some criticisms of using dichotomous scoring in IQ tests?"

AI (overview) response: There are several significant criticisms of using dichotomous scoring (correct/incorrect) in IQ tests and other psychological assessments. These issues primarily revolve around the loss of valuable data, the encouragement of less-than-ideal test-taking behavior, and a simplification of complex cognitive processes.

Loss Of Information

  • Ignores partial knowledge: Dichotomous scoring treats a test-taker who gets a problem almost right the same as one who is completely wrong. For example, on a multiple-response item, a student who selects two out of three correct options and none of the incorrect ones receives a zero, just like a student who selected no correct options at all. This discards information about the individual's level of partial understanding.
  • Oversimplifies cognitive performance: By reducing a response to a simple 0 or 1, this scoring method loses all nuance. It does not reflect the process of arriving at an answer, the student's reasoning, or their qualitative understanding of the subject matter.
  • Decreases statistical power: For researchers, dichotomizing continuous data (like raw scores) into binary categories results in a loss of information and statistical power. This can make it harder to detect true effects and may necessitate larger sample sizes to compensate.
,br> Effects on test-taking behavior

  • Encourages guessing: Multiple-choice questions scored dichotomously allow for correct answers based purely on luck. A test-taker with a low level of knowledge can guess and potentially achieve a higher score than their understanding warrants. The probability of guessing a correct answer increases as the number of choices decreases, undermining the assessment's accuracy.
  • Disincentivizes effort on "wrong" answers: Because a partially correct answer is scored the same as a completely incorrect one, there is no incentive for a test-taker to demonstrate partial understanding.

Flawed interpretation of results

  • Arbitrary cut-off points: For continuous variables like raw scores, dichotomous classification requires an arbitrary cut-off point to be established. An individual just below the cut-off is categorized as "different" from someone just above it, even though their actual performance might be very similar. This can lead to misclassification, as demonstrated by studies showing that two similar assessment methods can fail to classify the same individuals as "impaired" when using a binary cut-off.
  • Inappropriate for dimensional constructs: Many human traits, including intelligence, are dimensional, not categorical. A dichotomous (e.g., "impaired" vs. "not impaired") or even trichotomous classification system can force dimensional traits into overly simplistic categories, resulting in "procrustean distortions" that ignore the complexity of the individual.

Alternative scoring methods

To address these criticisms, some tests incorporate more nuanced scoring methods:
  • Partial-credit scoring: Awards credit for responses that demonstrate a degree of partial understanding. For instance, on a multiple-response item, credit is given for each correct option selected, rather than requiring all correct options to be selected for any credit. This increases score variability and reliability.
  • Item Response Theory (IRT): This statistical method models the probability of a test-taker with a certain ability level answering an item correctly. It provides a more accurate estimate of a test-taker's ability and can be used to score items with partial credit, but it requires a larger dataset and complex analysis.
  • Rubric-based evaluation: For constructed-response items, rubrics allow trained raters to score responses based on criteria beyond a simple right/wrong answer. This addresses qualitative aspects of a test-taker's performance that dichotomous scoring would miss.


[3] Internet query: "What are some criticisms of using trichotomous scoring in IQ tests?"

AI (overview) response: A key criticism of trichotomous scoring, which categorizes answers as correct, incorrect, or "not reached" (treated as unadministered), is that it can produce biased and unreliable test results. This method has significant limitations, particularly in educational and diagnostic settings, by failing to account for the reasons a test-taker did not answer a question.

Problems caused by a "not reached" classification

  • Information loss and misrepresentation: Scoring "not reached" items as not-administered reduces the overall data available, which can lead to a less precise measurement of an individual's true ability.
  • Artificial inflation or deflation of scores: The method can cause biased estimates of a test-taker's abilities and the item's difficulty.
    • Treating unanswered items as incorrect unfairly penalizes slow-working test-takers, potentially deflating their scores.
    • Ignoring unanswered items might inflate the scores of those who skip difficult questions, as they are not penalized for the missed items.
  • Discouragement of honest test-taking: If test-takers understand the scoring mechanism, they may strategically skip difficult items to maximize their score, rather than attempting every question honestly. This subverts the test's purpose of measuring ability.
  • Reduced effectiveness for different ability levels: For assessments with many "not reached" items, ignoring those items can make the test informative only for high-ability students. This limits the test's diagnostic and educational value for those with lower ability.

Flaws related to underlying assumptions

The broader context of IQ test criticism extends to fundamental issues with how test-makers assume intelligence is measured.
  • Oversimplification of intelligence: Critiques of IQ tests argue that they measure only a narrow range of cognitive skills, often ignoring other important factors of intelligence like social, creative, and emotional skills.
  • Flawed interpretation of results: IQ test results do not necessarily indicate a fixed potential. A student's performance can be affected by factors other than innate ability, including exposure to the tested content, test anxiety, or other motivational states during the assessment.
  • Neglect of the test-taking process: IQ tests that focus only on the final, "correct" answer fail to consider the cognitive process used to arrive at a solution. This can misinterpret the ability of test-takers who use alternative but equally valid reasoning.
  • Cultural and socioeconomic bias: IQ tests, including their scoring methods, have been criticized for favoring test-takers from white, middle-class backgrounds. For instance, someone from a different cultural background may approach a problem differently and be unfairly penalized because their reasoning differs from the test-maker's assumption.


This section is the "Deeper Dive" option of the same query. (It uses a dualistically (two-patterned) arranged assignment of the information.)

AI response: In intelligence testing, trichotomous scoring categorizes a test-taker's response into one of three outcomes: correct, incorrect, or partially correct. While it is more nuanced than simple dichotomous (right/wrong) scoring, it faces several criticisms related to subjectivity, reliability, and validity.

Limitations and issues with trichotomous scoring

Potential for subjectivity and bias
  • Arbitrary classification: Critics argue that assigning partial credit can be subjective and vary between test administrators. The criteria for what constitutes a "partially correct" answer are not always clearly defined, potentially leading to inconsistencies in scoring.
  • Reinforcing existing biases: IQ tests are already criticized for cultural and socioeconomic bias, and the addition of a subjective scoring category could exacerbate this problem. A test-taker from a different cultural background may arrive at a correct or partially correct answer using a different, but equally valid, reasoning process that is not accounted for in the scoring rubric.

Reduced reliability
  • Inter-rater reliability: If different scorers apply different standards for partial credit, the same test performance could yield different scores depending on who administers it. This reduces the test's inter-rater reliability, which is the degree of agreement among independent raters.
  • Test-retest reliability: A test-taker's score might fluctuate on repeated tests not because of a change in their ability, but because of inconsistent application of partial credit. This undermines the test-retest reliability, which measures the consistency of a measure over time.

Questionable validity
  • Process versus product: Traditional IQ tests are often criticized for measuring only the product of intelligence (the final answer) rather than the complex cognitive processes used to arrive at that answer. Trichotomous scoring adds a layer of complexity but still does not fully address the issue of evaluating cognitive strategies.
  • False precision: While providing more detail than dichotomous scoring, the "partially correct" category may offer a false sense of precision about a test-taker's ability. A single partially correct answer may not provide enough information to accurately represent a person's cognitive strengths and weaknesses, especially compared to more comprehensive diagnostic tools.

Insufficiently informative
  • Information loss: For certain items, simply classifying a response as "partially correct" might not capture the full range of a test-taker's cognitive processes. Polytomous scoring, which uses a scale with multiple points (e.g., a 0–6 rubric), is often a better alternative for capturing a more nuanced performance.
  • Limited utility: While adding detail compared to dichotomous scoring, the limited three-point scale may not be sufficient for clinical diagnosis or for informing instructional processes. In some cases, a more detailed analysis is needed to determine the specific nature of a learning difficulty or cognitive profile.

Potential for misinterpretation
  • Misidentifying ability: If a test-taker is prone to guessing, a trichotomous system could inaccurately assign partial credit, leading to a misrepresentation of their true ability. Research has shown that sometimes a test-taker with a wrong answer may have a similar level of understanding as someone who gets the answer right.
  • Flooring effects: At the low end of the performance spectrum, the "partially correct" category may not provide enough distinction between different levels of low performance, especially for those with intellectual disabilities. This can lead to a "flooring effect" where important variability in ability is lost.


[4] Internet query: "Tripartite Aspects of the 3 historical puzzles used to test rationality"

AI (overview) response: The query does not refer to a single, historically accepted set of three puzzles used to test rationality. Instead, the concept likely derives from the structure of one of the most famous and difficult logic problems ever posed: the "Hardest Logic Puzzle Ever," also known as the Three Gods puzzle. Its Tripartite nature stems from its three-part structure involving truth, falsehood, and randomness.

The Three Gods puzzle

First published by philosopher George Boolos in 1996, the puzzle's difficulty lies in the fact that the identities of the gods are unknown, the words for "yes" and "no" are unknown, and one of the gods gives random answers. The Tripartite aspects of the puzzle are:
  • The Three Gods: The core challenge involves interrogating three entities: True (always truthful), False (always lying), and Random (answers randomly).
  • The Tripartite Questioning Strategy: To solve the puzzle, one must construct a specific, three-part line of questioning. The first question is designed to identify a god who is not Random. The second question reveals if the non-random god is True or False. The final question then identifies the Random god.
  • The Three Layers of Uncertainty: The puzzle tests rational thought by layering three levels of uncertainty that must be overcome through logical deduction:
    • Uncertain Identities: Which god is which?
    • Uncertain Language: Which word means "yes" and which means "no"?
    • Uncertain Behavior: How to manage the unpredictability of the Random god?

Other famous rationality tests

The request may be broadly referring to three distinct categories of famous rationality puzzles, including paradoxes and induction puzzles, that collectively test different aspects of rational thinking:

1. The Monty Hall Problem

This probability puzzle is famous for demonstrating how counter-intuitive rational decisions can be for humans.
  • Three Aspects: The puzzle involves three doors: one with a car and two with goats.
  • Test of Rationality: It tests a person's ability to rationally re-evaluate probabilities after new information is revealed. The correct answer (switching doors) defies most people's initial instinct.
  • Tripartite Nature: The problem centers on the three choices (the door you pick, the door the host opens, and the third door you can switch to).

2. The Muddy Children Problem
This induction puzzle tests how a group of rational agents reason about each other's knowledge.
  • Three Aspects: While typically involving more than three children, the problem is often presented as three children with muddy foreheads who must deduce if their own forehead is dirty.
  • Test of Rationality: It tests common knowledge and higher-order reasoning. The children must not only be rational but also assume that everyone else is also rational.
  • Tripartite Nature: The problem relies on a three-stage process: a publicly known piece of information, a subsequent shared observation, and a series of deductions that leads to a simultaneous solution.

3. The River Crossing Puzzle

This classic logic puzzle challenges rational sequential planning.
  • Three Aspects: In the famous version published by Alcuin of York, a farmer must transport a wolf, a goat, and a cabbage across a river.
  • Test of Rationality: The puzzle tests the ability to apply a set of rules (the wolf and goat cannot be left alone, nor can the goat and cabbage) to devise a step-by-step rational plan.
  • Tripartite Nature: It requires managing three distinct entities with three specific restrictions to achieve a three-part goal (moving all three items across the river safely).



Page initially created: Tuesday, Sept. 16th, 2025... 1:13 AM
Initial posting: Sunday, Sept. 21st, 2025... 2:09 PM